Study Shows Surprising Skill That Underlies Math and Reading

BirdsDo you remember second grade reading groups? They had nifty names like redbirds, bluebirds, and goldfinches to keep us from discovering who was in a “high” group and who was in a “low” group. But it took about four seconds to realize that the redbirds in reading were the same kids as the redbirds in math, solving the easy mystery of the bird groups. And this points to a great, unfair truth in education: Smart kids tend to be smart across the board, whereas kids who struggle in reading tend to also struggle in math. Why is that? These skills seem so distinct! Is it general intelligence that boosts both? Study skills? Tiger parenting?

Of course, not everybody who is good at math is also good at reading (and vice versa). But there’s an ability underlying these two skills and a paper by researchers at Vanderbilt University scheduled for publication in the journal Child Development pinpoints what it is.

First let’s take a look at the science. The Vanderbilt study is both ambitious and powerful, following 747 kids from first through third grades and testing them along the way to discover their paths. There’s some obvious stuff: kids who started with high reading skills usually finished with high reading skills. The same was true of math. (The authors point to this as evidence that a child’s academic trajectory can be largely fixed very early…)

But then there’s a whole mix of skills that help math, reading or both. The skill that helps both is not one thing, and it’s not exactly the mix of general intelligence. Instead, it’s memory retrieval. This skill of memory retrieval is made up of sub-skills including attentive behavior, reasoning, working memory, visuospatial memory, and something called rapid automatized naming (basically, how quickly you can recall the word “apple” when presented with a picture of an apple). Basically, the skill that makes kids good or bad at both math and reading is the ability to pack and unpack memory.

“This suggests that competent third-grade calculation and word-reading performance both rely on the ability to form and fluently retrieve from memory arbitrary associations between the visual symbolic and phonological forms,” the paper writes. This was especially true for math skills: This One Skill To Rule Them All trumped the influence of any single skill on the development of math ability.

Here’s an example of how this works: attentive behavior does not directly predict which kids will be good at reading. But this attentive behavior tends to boost kids’ memory retrieval, which in turn boosts reading. If somehow attentive behavior didn’t help a child sow and reap from his or her memory, it wouldn’t necessarily do squat for his or her reading. (Yes, I realize that’s wonky…)

Then there are skills that even taken piecemeal boost reading or math. Here are the skills that boost reading: language, phonological memory, rapid automatized naming. Here are the skills that boost math: attentive behavior, working memory, reasoning. Notice that there is no overlap! Outside the underlying ability of memory retrieval, math and reading are different skills.

So now we’ve got two things going on. We’ve got a pool of skills that create memory retrieval, boosting both reading and math together, and we’ve got individual skills that boost one or the other but not both. We’ve also got at least a preliminary answer to a longstanding question in education: why do so many kids do well or poorly in both reading and math, but also why do some kids break this mold?

The answer is that kids who are good at both very likely depend on this skill of memory retrieval–this high tide floats both boats. Kids who are good only at reading might be a little lower in memory retrieval (allowing them to under perform in math), but probably have one or more of the special reading skills of language, phonological memory, or rapid automatized naming. Likewise, kids who are only good at math might lack the One Skill To Rule Them All, but have special prowess in attentive behavior, working memory, and/or reasoning.

In a vastly oversimplified but nonetheless compelling and thought-provoking nutshell, that’s why most kids are good or bad at both reading and math, but some kids can buck the trend to excel at only one

Facebooktwitterredditpinterestlinkedinmailby feather
This entry was posted in Brain, Education, Intelligence, Math, parenting, Science and tagged , , , , , . Bookmark the permalink.

One Response to Study Shows Surprising Skill That Underlies Math and Reading

  1. Interesting article. I taught mathematics for over 20 years at a large urban high school; many of my students were English learners. Whether the class was algebra, geometry, trigonometry, or calculus, my primary teaching tools included reading and writing. In my experience, proficiency in reading was linked to proficiency in mathematics.

    P. S. I am so glad to see someone writing about mathematics. I will check out your books.

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *